Read full paper at:
http://www.scirp.org/journal/PaperInformation.aspx?PaperID=52838#.VKoBpMnQrzE
http://www.scirp.org/journal/PaperInformation.aspx?PaperID=52838#.VKoBpMnQrzE
Author(s)
1Research on Natural and Artificial Intelligence (RANI), Jundiaí, Brazil.
2School of Medicine, University of São Paulo Brazil, São Paulo, Brazil.
3London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK.
4Instituto de Pesquisas em Teoria da Informação (IPTI), Aracajú, Brazil.
5Laboratory of Signal Processing, Department od Electrical Engeneering, Faculdade de Engenharia Industrial, São Paulo, Brazil.
6Escola Paulista de Medicina, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil.
2School of Medicine, University of São Paulo Brazil, São Paulo, Brazil.
3London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK.
4Instituto de Pesquisas em Teoria da Informação (IPTI), Aracajú, Brazil.
5Laboratory of Signal Processing, Department od Electrical Engeneering, Faculdade de Engenharia Industrial, São Paulo, Brazil.
6Escola Paulista de Medicina, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil.
Brazil has introduced a referendum regarding the
prohibition of firearm commerce and propaganda arguments have invoked
socially and personally driven issues in the promotion of voting in
favor of and against firearm control, respectively. Here, we used
different techniques to study the brain activity associated with a
voter’s perception of the truthfulness of these arguments and their
influence on voting decisions. Low-resolution tomography was used to
identify the possible different sets of neurons activated in the
analysis of the different types of propaganda. Linear correlation was
used to calculate the amount information H(ei)
provided to different electrodes about how these sets of neurons enroll
themselves to carry out this cognitive analysis. The results clearly
showed that vote decision was not influenced by arguments that were
introduced by propaganda, which was typically driven by specific social
or self-interest motives. However, different neural circuits were
identified in the analysis of each type of propaganda argument,
independently of the declared vote (for or against the control)
intention.
KEYWORDS
Cite this paper
da Rocha, A. , Massad, E. , Rocha, F. and
Burattini, M. (2014) Brain and Law: An EEG Study of How We Decide or
Not to Implement a Law. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 4, 559-578. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2014.412054.
[1] |
Rocha, A.F. (2013) Toward a
Better Understanding of the Relationship between Neurosciences and Law.
Law & Neuroscience Journal, 4. http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2313182 |
[2] | Hobbes, T. (1651) Leviathan. Penguin Classics, Penguin Books, New York. |
[3] | Rousseau, J.J. (1968) Du Contrat Social: Texte Original. Penguin Books Limited, London. |
[4] | Hart, H.L.A. (1961) Concept of Law. Oxford University Press, Oxford. |
[5] | Kelsen, H. (1960) Reine Rechteslehre. Verlag Fraz Deuticke, Viena. |
[6] |
Rocha, A.F., Theoto, F.,
Burattini, M.N. and Massad, E. (2010) Neurodynamics of an Election.
Brain Research, 1351, 198-211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.046 |
[7] |
Frith, C.D. and Frith, U. (2006) The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Neuron, 50, 531-534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 |
[8] |
Cavanna, A.R. and Trimble, M.R.
(2006) The Precuneus: A Review of Its Functional Anatomy and Behavioral
Correlates. Brain, 129, 564-583. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl004 |
[9] |
Hartwright, C.E., Apperly, I.A.
and Hansen, P.C. (2006) Multiple Roles for Executive Control in
Belief-Desire Reasoning: Distinct Neural Networks Are Recruited for Self
Perspective Inhibition and Complexity of Reasoning. NeuroImage, 61,
921-930. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.012 |
[10] |
Van den Ouden, H.E.M., Frith,
U., Frith, C. and Blakemore, S.-J. (2005) Thinking about Intentions.
NeuroImage, 28, 787-796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.001 |
[11] | Pasqual-Marqui, R.D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K. and Lehmann, D. (2002) Functional Imaging with Low-Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA): A Review. Methods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, 24, 91-95. |
[12] |
Rocha, F.T., Rocha, A.F.,
Massad, E. and Menezes, R.X. (2005) Brain Mappings of the Arithmetic
Processing in Children and Adults. Cognitive Brain Research, 22,
359-372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.008 |
[13] |
Daw, N.D., O’Doherty, J.P.,
Dayan, P., Seymor, B. and Dolan, R.J. (2006) Cortical Substrates for
Exploratory Decisions in Humans. Nature, 441, 876-879. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04766 |
[14] |
Addis, D.R. and Schacter, D.L.
(2008) Constructive Episodic Simulation: Temporal Distance and Detail of
Past and Future Events Modulate Hippocampal Engagement. Hippocampus,
18, 227-237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20405 |
[15] | Cabeza, R., Dolcos, F., Graham, R. and Nyberg, L. (2002) Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory. Neuroimagen, 16, 317-330. |
[16] |
Lepage, M., Ghaffar, O., Nyberg,
L. and Tulving, E. (2000) Prefrontal Cortex and Episodic Memory
Retrieval Mode Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 97, 506-511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.1.506 |
[17] |
Platel, H., Baron, J.-C.,
Desgranges, B., Bernard, F. and Eustache, F. (2003) Semantic and
Episodic Memory of Music Are Subserved by Distinct Neural Networks.
NeuroImage, 20, 244-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00287-8 |
[18] |
Ranganath, C., Johnson, M.K. and
D’ésposito, M. (2003) Prefrontal Activity Associated with Working
Memory and Episodic Long-Term Memory. Neuropsychologia, 41, 378-389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00169-0 |
[19] |
Rugg, M.D., Fletcher, P.C.,
Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J. and Dolan, R.J. (1996) Differential
Activation of the Prefrontal Cortex in Successful and Unsuccessful
Memory Retrieval. Brain, 119, 2073-2083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.6.2073 |
[20] |
Baron, J.C. and Jones, T. (2012)
Oxygen Metabolism, Oxygen Extraction and Positron Emission Tomography:
Historical Perspective and Impact on Basic and Clinical Neuroscience.
Neuroimage, 61, 492-504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.036 |
[21] |
Scheeringa, R., Petersson, K.M.,
Oostenveld, R., Norris, D.G., Hagoort, P. and Bastiaansen, M.C.M.
(2009) Trial-by-Trial Coupling between EEG and BOLD Identifies Networks
Related to Alpha and Theta EEG Power Increases during Working Memory
Maintenance NeuroImage, 44, 1224-1238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.041 |
[22] |
Canessa, N., Gorini, A., Cappa,
S.F., Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Danna, M., Fazio, F. and Perani, D.
(2005) The Effect of Social Content on Deductive Reasoning: An fMRI
Study. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 30-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20114 |
[23] |
Henson, R.N.A., Shallice, T. and
Dolan, R.J. (1999) Right Prefrontal Cortex and Episodic
Memoryretrieval: A Functional MRI Test of the Monitoring Hypothesis.
Brain, 122, 1367-1381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.7.1367 |
[24] |
Knauff, K., Mulack, T.,
Kassubek, J., Salih, H.R. and Greenlee, M.W. (2002) Spatial Imagery in
Deductive Reasoning: A Functional MRI Study. Cognitive Brain Research,
13, 203-212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00116-1 |
[25] |
Goel, V. and Dolan, R.J. (2001)
Functional Neuroanatomy of Three-Term Relational Reasoning.
Neuropsychologia, 39, 901-909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00024-0 |
[26] | Mackey, A.P., Miller Singley, A.T. and Bunge, S.A. (2013) Intensive Reasoning Training Alters Patterns of Brain Connectivity at Rest. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4796-4803. |
[27] |
Vollm, B.A., Taylor, A.N.W.,
Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., et al. (2006)
Neuronal Correlates of Theory of Mind and Empathy: A Functional Magnetic
Resonance Imaging Study in a Nonverbal Task. NeuroImage, 29, 90-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.022 |
[28] |
Lundstrom, B.N., Ingvar, M. and
Petersson, K.M. (2005) The Role of Percuneus and Frontal Cortex during
Source Memory Episodic Retrieval. NeuroImage, 27, 824-834. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.008 |
[29] |
Van der Meer, L., Groenewold,
N.A., Nolen, W.A., Pijnenborg, M. and Aleman, A. (2011) Inhibit Yourself
and Understand the Other: Neural Basis of Distinct Processes Underlying
Theory of Mind. NeuroImage, 56, 2364-2374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.053 |
[30] |
Martino, J., Gabarrós, A., Deus,
J., Juncadella, M., Acebes, J.J., Torres, A. and Pujol, J. (2011)
Intrasurgical Mapping of Complex Motor Function in the Superior Frontal
Gyrus. Neuroscience, 179, 131-142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.047 |
[31] |
Nakagawa, S., Nishiike, S.,
Tonoike, M., Takeda, N. and Kubo, T. (2002) Measurements of Brain
Magnetic Fields Associated with Apparent Self-Motion. International
Congress Series, 1232, 367-371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0531-5131(01)00790-7 |
[32] |
Samson, D., Apperly, I.A.,
Chiavarino, C. and Humphreys, G.W. (2004) Left Tempoparietal Junction Is
Necessary for Representing Someone Else’s Belief. Nature Neuroscience,
7, 499-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1223 |
[33] |
Sulpizio, V., Committeri, G.,
Lambrey, S., Berthoz, A. and Galati, G. (2013) Selective Role of
Lingual/Parahippocampal Gyrus and Retrosplenial Complex in Spatial
Memory across Viewpoint Changes Relative to the Environmental Reference
Frame. Behavioral Brain Research, 242, 62-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.12.031 |
[34] |
Bullmore, E. and Sporns, O.
(2009) Complex Brain Networks: Graph Theoretical Analysis of Structural
and Functional Systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 186-198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575 eww1510105lx |
评论
发表评论